A University Grammar of English Textbook
A University Grammar of English Textbook
Sidney Greenbaum
Geoffrey Leech
Jan Svartvik
A University
Grammar of
English
Longman
CONTENTS
PREFACE............................................................................................. 11
SYMBOLS AND TECHNICAL CONVENTIONS..................... 14
ONE
Varieties of English............................................................................. 15
Standard English 15; National standards o f English 16
TWO
Elements of grammar............................................................................. 18
Sentence elements 20; Parts o f speech 25;
Stative and dynamic 28;
Pro-forms, question, and negation 29
THREE
Verbs and the verb phrase .............................................................. 32
Regular verbs and inflectional rules 33,
Irregular verbs 35; Auxiliaries 36;
Tense and aspect 40; Mood 50
FOUR
Nouns, pronouns, and the basic noun phrase.................................... 58
Noun classes 58; Determiners 60; Reference
and the articles 66;
Number 76; Gender 85; The genitive 89;
Pronouns 95
FIVE
Adjectives and adverbs...............................................................................108
Adjectives, attributive and predicative 109,
Adverbs as modifiers 119;
Comparison and intensification 123;
Correspondence between adjective and
adverb 128, Adjective, adverb, and other
word-classes 129
SIX
Prepositions and prc^positional phrases.............................................. 134
Place relations 137 Time relations 144;
Other relations chiefly as adjunct 146,
as disjunct 151, as complementation
o f verb or adjective 152
SEVEN
The simple sentence....................................................................................154
Clause patterns 154; Elements and
their meanings 158; Concord 163;
Negation 170; Questions 177;
Commands 184
EIGHT
Adjuncts, disjuncts, conjuncts . ^............................................................... 189
Limiter and additive adjuncts 193; Intensifiers 195;
Adjuncts o f manner, means, and instrument 201,
o f place 205, o f time 209; Style and
attitudinal disjuncts 219; Conjuncts 221
12
TWO
ELEMENTS OF GRAMMAR
2.1
Parts of a sentence
2.2
Subject and predicate
2.3
Operator, auxiliary, and predication
In contrast with the subject, there are few generalizations that we can
usefully make about the predicate since - as our examples have illustrat
ed — it tends to be a more complex and heterogeneous unit. We need
to subdivide it into its elements or constituents. One division has
already been suggested; this distinguishes A U X ILIA RY as O PERA TO R
(as in [5q]) from what we may call the PR ED IC A TIO N . The dis
tinctions may be illustrated as follows:
sentence
subject predicate
I
I auxiliary predication
I as
I operato r I
I
I I
I I
had given the girl an apple [5J
HaTiie given the girl an apple? [5q]
This particular division o f the sentence helps us to understand, for
example, how interrogative and negative sentences are formed, how cer
tain adjuncts are positioned, and how certain types of emphasis are
achieved.
2.4
Range of operators
2.5
Sentence elements
object
[mdirect object (Oi)
complement complement (C )
[object complement (Co)
2.8
But distinctions between verbs need to be drawn not only in relation
to object- and complement-types but also in relation to whether they
themselves admit the aspectual contrast of ‘progressive’ and ‘non-pro-
gressive’. Thus it is possible to say
John carefully searched the room [1]
or John was carefully searching the room
It rained steadily all day [4]
or It was raining steadily all day
But it is not possible to use the progressive in
The girl is now a student at a large university [2]
*The girl is now being a student ...
John knew the answer [10]
*John was knowing the answer
When verbs (either habitually or in certain uses) will not admit the
progressive, as in [2] and [10], they are called STATIVE. When they
will adm it it, as in [1] and [4], they are called DYN A M IC. It is
normal for verbs to be dynamic and even the minority that are almost
always stative can usually be given a dynamic use on occasion.
2.9
Categories of adverbial
Next we may take a preliminary look at adverbials, concerning ourselves
only with such distinctions as are necessary to explain some o f the chief
restrictions in constructing the simplest sentences. We may begin by
looking again at a sentence with two adverbials:
The girl is now a student at a large university
22
This might have had fewer elements:
The girl is a student at a large university
The girl is a student
The girl is now a student
The girl is at a large university
but the sentence could not have been formed initially as:
*The girl is now
On this evidence we may say that the adverbials now and at a large
university belong to different classes and it seems natural to label them
‘time’ and ‘place’ respectively.
Consider now the fact that the adverbial carefully in illustration [1]
could be replaced by many others, making acceptable sentences in
each case:
carefully
slowly
John searched the room noisily
sternly
w ithout delay
But if these same adverbials were inserted in sentences which had stative
verbs, the sentences would become unacceptable:
♦carefully
*slowly
The girl is now a student ...
*noisily
John knew the answer ...
*stemly
*without delay
2 .10
intens: Cs [4] (A (A
tim e)
#mono: Od [5] (A place)
Vd y n trans^ di:(O i ) Oj [6] process)
ext
complex: O dQ [71
in tran sitiv e [81
2 .1 1
Parts of speech
2.12
The structures realizing sentence elements are composed of units which
can be referred to as parts o f speech. These can be exemplified for
English as follows:
(a) noun — John, room, answer, play
adjective — happy, steady, new, large, round
adverb — steadily, completely, really, very, then
verb — search, grow, play, be, have, do
25
' (b) article — the, a(n)
demonstrative — that, this
pronoun — he, they, anybody, one, which
preposition — of, at, in, without, in spite of
conjunction — and, that, when, although
interjection — oh, ah, ugh, phew
We should notice that the examples are listed as words intheir
‘dictionary form ’ and not as they often appear in sentences when they
function as constituents of phrases: thus the singular room and not the
plural rooms, the simple happy and not the comparative happier, the
infinitive (or uninflected) grow and not the past grew, the subject form
he and not the object form him.
Note
From even the few examples given, it can be seen that a part-of-speech item may
consist o f more than a single word. This is especially common in the case of complex
prepositions, such as in spite of, out of.
2.13
Some of the examples in 2.12 appear as more than one part of speech
{play as noun and verb, that as demonstrative and conjunction) and
more of them could have been given additional entries in this way
(round can be noun, verb, adjective, adverb, and preposition). Similar
ly, we should notice a direct correspondence between most adjectives
and adverbs, the latter usually consisting o f the former plus -ly. Less
obviously, there is an im portant correspondence between all words beginning
/3/ {the, that, then, for example) and many o f those beginning wh-
{which, when, for example): basically the form er are relater or indicator
words and the latter interrogative words.
2.14
Closed-system items
The parts of speech in 2.12 are listed in two groups, (a) and (b),
and this introduces a distinction o f very great significance. Set (b)
comprises w hat are called ‘closed-system’ items. T hat is, the sets of
items are closed in the sense that they cannot normally be extended
by the creation of additional members: a m om ent’s reflection is enough
for us to realize how rarely in a language we invent or adopt a new
o r additional pronoun. It requires no great effort to list all the members
in a closed system, and to be reasonably sure that one has in fact
made an exhaustive inventory (especially, o f course, where the membership
is so extremely small as in the case o f the article).
The items are said to constitute a system in being (i) reciprocally
exclusive: the decision to use one item in a given structure excludes
the possibility of using any other (thus one can have the book or
a book but not the book)', and (ii) reciprocally defining: it is
26
less easy to state the meaning o f any individual item than to define
it in relation to the rest of the system. This may be clearer with
a non-linguistic analogy. If we are told that a student came third
in an examination, the ‘meaning’ that we attach to ‘third’ will depend
on knowing how many candidates took the exam ination: ‘third’ in a set
o f four has a very different meaning from ‘third’ in a set of thirty.
2.15
Open-class items
2.16
But we saw in 2.8 that there were some verbs such as know
which could not normally be used with the progressive (*he is know
ing) : that is, which could not be seen as referring to something
that was in progress. Verbs so used we called ‘stative’, and they
should be seen as exceptions within the class o f verbs. There are
exceptions in the other direction among the nouns, not all of which
need be stative. For example, a child may be well-behaved one minute
and a nuisance the next. The situation is similar when we turn to
the remaining open word-class, adjectives. Although they are predom i
nantly stative {tall, red, old), some adjectives can resemble verbs in
referring on occasion to transitory conditions of behaviour or activity
such as naughty or insolent. And since be must be used to make
predications having any noun or adjective as complement, we must
qualify the statement made in 2.8 that this is a stative verb: it can
28
also be used dynamically, in the progressive, when the complement is
dynamic:
u . , fa nuisance]
|„ .„ g h ty r * “ "
2.17
Pro-forms
The names o f the parts of speech are traditional, however, and neither
in themselves nor in relation to each other do these names give a
safe guide to their meaning, which instead is best understood in terms
o f their grammatical properties. ‘Adverb’ is a classic instance. We have
seen some justification in the previous section for ‘participle’, ahd of course
the ‘pronoun’ is an even clearer exception in correctly suggesting that
it can serve as a replacement for a noun:
John searched the big room and the small one [1]
There are pro-form s also for place, time, and other adverbials under
certain circumstances:
Mary is in London and John is there too [3]
Mary arrived on Tuesday and John arrived then too [4]
John searched the big room very carefully and the sijiall
one less so ' [5]
Here do so replaces all the italicized portion, the head verb search
and the rest o f the predication, as is shown below:
29
sentence
subject predicate
I
I auxiliary predication
I as
o p erato r I
I I
I
. . .h e would search the room carefully ,
...he didn’t do so
2.20
Negation and non-assertion
{
assertion — positive and declarative
J interrogative
non-assertion [negative
3.1
Types of verb
There are various ways in which it will be necessary to classify verbs
in this chapter. We begin with a classification relating to the function
of items in the verb phrase. This distinguishes lexical verbs from the
closed system o f auxiliary verbs, and subdivides the latter into primary
and modal auxiliaries.
LEXICAL walk, write, play, beautify, etc.
A U X ILIA R Y \ „ ,,
[ M odal can, may, shall, will,
could, might, should, would,
must, ought to, used to, need, dare
Note
As we shall see (3.22), some of the modals listed differ in their inflectional and syntactic
behaviour from others and will be referred to as ‘marginal’. On the other hand, further
items like had better or tend to could be added to the list since they have a similar
semantic relation in the verb phrase to the modals; these other expressions are often
called ‘semi-auxiliaries’.
3.2
Verbal forms and the verb phrase
Many English verbs have five forms: the BASE, the -S FO RM , the PAST,
the ^ING PA R TIC IPLE, and the ^ED PA R TIC IPLE. Regular lexical
verbs have the same -ed inflection for both the past tense and the
-ed participle (called). Irregular lexical verb forms vary from three
(eg: put, puts, putting) to eight (be, am, is, are, was, were, being,
been). The modal auxiliaries are defective in not having infinitive (*to
may), -ing participle (*maying), -ed participle (*mayed), or imperative
(*may\).
These are regular in that we can predict the other forms if we know
the base of such a verb. This is a very powerful rule, since the base is
the form listed in dictionaries and the vast majority of English verbs
belong to this regular class. Furtherm ore, all new verbs that are coined
or borrowed from other languages adopt this pattern.
3.5
The ‘-ing’ and ‘-s’ forms
The -ing form is a straightforward addition to the base:
push pushing sleep ^ sleeping
The -s form is also predictable from the base. It has three spoken
realizations: /iz/, /z/, and /s/, and two spellings, -s and -es.
(1) Pronounced /iz/ after bases ending in voiced or voiceless sibilants
and spelled -es unless the base already ends in -e, eg
pass ^ passes budge ^ budges '
buzz buzzes push pushes
catch catches camouflage ^ camouflages
(2) Pronounced /z/ and spelled -s after bases ending in other voiced
sounds, eg
call ^ calls rob ^ robs flow flows
N ote: do ^ does go ^ goes
say says have has
(3) Pronounced /s/ and spelled -s after bases ending in other voiceless
sounds, eg
cut cuts lock ^ locks sap ^ saps
3.6
The past and the ‘-ed’ participle
The past (y-ed{) and the -ed participle (У-ed^ of regular Verbs (spelled
-ed unless the base ends in -e) have three spoken realizations:
2 P. Кверк и др. 33
/id/ after bases ending in /d / and /t/, eg
pad ^ padded pat ^ patted
/d/ after bases ending in voiced sounds other than /d/, eg
mow ^ mowed budge budged
III after bases ending in voiceless sounds other than /t/, eg
pass ^ passed pack packed
EX CEPTIO N S:
(a) Bases ending in certain consonants are doubled also after single
unstressed vowels: -g -gg-, -c -ck-:
humbug humbugging humbugged
traffic trafficking trafficked
(b) BrE, as distinct from AmE, breaks the rule with respect to
certain other consonants also: -/ -► -w -> -mm-, -p -► -pp-:
signal signalling signalled (BrE) 1
signal signaling signaled (AmE)J
travel travelling travelled (BrE) 1
travel traveling traveled (AmE)J
program(me) programming programmed (BrE) I
program programing programed (AmE)J
worship worshipping worshipped (BrE) |
worship worshiping worshiped (AmE)J
3.9
Deletion of ‘-e’
Final -e is regularly dropped before the -ing and -ed inflections:
shave shaving shaved
Verbs with bases in -ee, -ye, -oe, and often ~ge are exceptions to
this rule in that they do not drop the -e before Ang\ but they do
drop it before -ed, as do also forms in -ie (tie tied):
-ее: agree agreeing agreed
-ye: dye dyeing dyed
-oe: hoe hoeing hoed
-ge: singe singeing singed
In many cases, there are prefixed verbs having the same inflections,
eg: outdo beside do.
Do
Do as lexical verb (‘perform ’, etc) and as pro-verb has the full range
of forms, including the present participle doing and the past participle
done:
W hat have you been doing today?
A : You said you would finish it. B: I have done so.
3.12 \
Have
Both as lexical verb and as auxiliary, have has the following forms:
36
Note
In the stative sense o f possession, have is often (especially in BrE) constructed as an auxiliary.
AmE prefers the ¿lo-construction:
J {haven’t 1
any books
( don V have J
In dynamic sense’s (receive, take, experience, etc), lexica! have in both AmE and BrE
normally has the ¿lo-construction:
Does he have coffee with his breakfast?
D id you have any difficulty getting here?
The ifo-construction is required in such expressions as
Did you have a good time?
There is also the informal have got, where have is constructed as an auxiliary, which
is frequently preferred (especially in BrE) as an alternative to have. It is particularly
common in negative and interrogative sentences. As a further alternative for expressing
negation, we have the negative determiner no:
I haven’t got any books I have no books
3.13
Be
The lexical and auxiliary verb be is unique among English verbs in
having eight different forms:
NON- UNCONTRACTED CON TRACTED
N E G A T IV E N E G A T IV E N E G A T IV E
base be
' 1st person am, w am not, *m not (aren't, ain't)
singular
3rd person
singular is, *s is not, 's not isn't
present
2nd person,
1st and 3rd
are, 're are not, re not aren't
person,
plural
37
Note
[a] Aren't I is widely used in BrE, but there is no generally acceptable contracted form
for am not in declarative sentences. Ain't is substandard in BrE and is so considered
by many in AmE; as well as serving as a contracted am not, it is used also for
isn't, aren’t, hasn’t, and haven’t.
\
[b\ The lexical verb be may have the i/o-construction in persuasive imperative sentences
and regularly has it with negative imperatives:
D o be quiet! D on’t be silly!
Note
[ii] M ayn’t is restricted to BrE, where it is rare.
[6] Shan’t is rare in AmE.
[c] Ought regularly has the io-infinitive, but AmE occasionally has the bare infinitive in
negative sentences and in questions (although should is commoner in both cases):
You oughtn’t smoke so much', Ought you smoke so muchl
3.15
Marginal modal auxiliaries
Used always takes the /o-infinitive and occurs only in the past tense.
It may take the ^^-construction, in which case the spellings d idnt
used .to and didn't use to both occur. The interrogative construction
used he to is especially BrE; did he used to is preferred in both
AmE and BrE.
Dare and need can be constructed either as modal auxiliaries (with
bare infinitive and with no inflected -s form) or as lexical verbs
(with /o-infinitive and with inflected -s form). The modal verb construc-
38
tion is restricted to non-assertive contexts, ie mainly negative and inter
rogative sentences, whereas the lexical verb construction can always be
used and is in fact the more common. Dare and need as auxiliaries
are probably rarer in AmE than in BrE.
M O D A L A U X IL IA R Y L E X IC A L V E R B
C O N S T R U C T IO N C O N S T R U C T IO N
Note
[a] Non-assertive forms are not confined to overtly negative and/or interrogative sentences
but can also be present in adverbials, eg\ He need do it only under these circumstances.
He need do it but once; in determiners, eg: He need have no fear. No soldier dare
disobey; in pronouns, eg\ No one dare predict or even implicitly, eg: All you
need do is, ... (‘You need do no more than
[¿] Blends of the two constructions are widely acceptable inthe case of dare: We do
not dare speak.
3.17
The modal, perfective, progressive and passive auxiliaries follow a strict
order in the complex verb phrase:
[I] M ODAL, always followed by an infinitive, as in
He would visit
[II] PER FECTIV E, always followed by an -ed form, as in
He had visited
He would have visited
[III] PROGRESSIVE, always followed by an -ing form, as in
He was visiting
He would have been visiting
[IV] PASSIVE, always followed by an -ed form, as in
He was visited
He would have been being visited
The last example is added for completeness but the full range of
auxiliaries is rarely found simultaneously in this way (though less rarely
with the get passive). Rather, it should be noted ¿lat, while the above
order is strictly followed, gaps are perfectly normal. For example:
I 4 - III: He may be visiting
II + IV : He has been visited
3.20
Present
The verbs keep {on), go on have a similar function to the normal progressive auxiliary be:
Past
3.21
A n action in the past may be seen
(1) as having taken place at a particular point o f time; or
(2) over a period; if the latter, the period may be seen as
(a) extending up to the present, or
(b) relating only to the past; if the latter, it may be viewed as
(i) having been completed, or as
(ii) not having been completed
( 2 a ) m m m m '/ A m --------------------------------------------------------------------------------
(2bi)
( 2 b i i ) - m m - - - ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Note
Past time can be expressed with present tense forms. The ‘historic present’ is fairly
common in vivid narrative:
At that moment, in comes a policeman
but has no such journalistic overtones with verbs o f communicating:
John tells me that there was a car accident last night
On the other hand, past tense forms need not refer to past time. ‘Did you want
to see me?’ is little more than a slightly politer version of ‘Do you ...?’
3.22
The past and the perfective
In relation to (2a), it is not the time specified in the sentence but
the period relevant to the time specified that m ust extend to the
present. Contrast
John lived, in Paris for ten years
(which entails that the* period of residence has come to an end and
which admits the possibility that John is dead) with
John has lived in Paris for ten years
which entails that John is still alive but permits the residence in
Paris to extend either to the present (the usual interpretation) or
to some unspecified date in the past. Com pare also:
For generations, Nepal has produced brilliant mountaineers
j iworked fVi
{have worked J I ^^B our
Note
There is some tendency (especially in AmE) to use the past informally in place o f the
perfective, as in / saw it already ( = ‘I have already seen it’).
3.23
Indefinite and definite
3.24
Past perfect
W hat was said o f the perfect in 3.21 — applies to the past perfect,
with the complication that the point o f current relevance to which the
past perfect extends is a point in the past:
Past P resent F u tu re
relevant point
________________ V __________________
(I say now \present] that) W hen I met him [relevant point in the past]
John had lived in Paris for ten years
In some contexts, the simple past and the past perfect are interchangeable; eg:
44
Неге the conjunction after is sufficient specification to indicate that the
arrival from the shopping expedition had taken place before the eating,
so that the extra time indication by means o f the past perfect becomes
redundant.
Note
There is no interchangeability when the past perfect is the past of the perfect:
John tells me that he hasn’t seen Mary since Monday
John told me that he hadn’t seen Mary since Monday
*John told me that he didn’t see Mary since Monday
3.25
The past and the progressive
As with the present, the progressive when used with the past speci
fies the limited duration o f an action:
I was writing with a special pen for a period last night but my
hand grew tired
In consequence, it is a convenient device to indicate a time span
within which another event (indicated by the simple past) can be seen as
taking place:
While I was writing, the phone rang
The ability to express .incom plete action with the progressive is
illustrated by the contrasting pair:
He read a book that evening (implies that he finished it)
He was reading a book that evening (implies that he did not finish it)
and more strikingly by:
The girl was drowning in the lake (will perm it ‘but someone dived
in and rescued her’)^
The girl drowned in the lake
Habitual activity may be expressed by the progressive provided it is
clear that the habit is'tem porary:
A t that time, we were bathing every day
and not merely sporadic:
*We were sometimes walking to the office
But general habits may be pejoratively referred to:
My brother was always losing his keys
3.26
The perfect progressive
Limited duration (or incompleteness) and current relevance can be jointly
expressed with the perfect progressive. Com pare:
45
He has eaten my chocolates (they are all gone)
He was eating my chocolates (but I stopped him)
He has been eating my chocolates (but there are some left)
Frequently the perfect progressive implies an especially recent activity,
the effects o f which are obvious, and the adverb ju st commonly
accompanies this usage:
It has rained a great deal since you were here
Oh look! It has just been raining ^
3.27
[A] D Y N A M IC
(1) Activity verbs: abandon, ask, beg, call, drink, eat, help, learn,
listen, look at, play, rain, read, say, slice, throw, whisper, work,
write, etc.
(2) Process verbs : change, deteriorate, grow, mature, slow down, widen,
etc. Both activity and process verbs are frequently used in
progressive aspect to indicate incomplete events in progress.
(3) Verbs o f bodily sensation (ache, feel, hurt, itch, etc) can have
either simple or progressive aspect with little difference in
meaning.
(4) Transitional event verbs (arrive, die, fall, land, leave, lose, etc)
occur in the progressive but with a change o f meaning compared
with simple aspect. The progressive implies inception, ie only
the approach to the transition.
(5) M om entary verbs (hit, jump, kick, knock, nod, tap, etc) have
little duration, and thus the progressive aspect powerfully
suggests repetition.
[B] STATIVE
(1) Verbs o f inert perception and cognition: abhor, adore, astonish,
believe, desire, detest, dislike, doubt, feel, forgive, guess, hate, hear,
imagine, impress, intend, know, like, love, mean, mind, perceive,
please, prefer, presuppose, realize, recall, recognize, regard, re
member, satisfy, see, smell, suppose, taste, think, understand,
want, wish, etc. Some o f these verbs may take other than a
recipient subject (7.11), in which case they belong with the A1
class. Com pare:
I think you are right [Bl]
I am thinking o f you all the time [Al]
46
(2) Relational verbs: apply to (everyone), be, belong to, concern,
consist of, contain, cost, depend on, deserve, equal, fit, have,
include, involve, lack, matter, need, owe, own, possess, remain
(a bachelor), require, resemble, seem, sound, suffice, tend, etc.
The future
3.28
There is no obvious future tense in English corresponding to the time/tense
relation for present and past. Instead there are several possibilities for
denoting future time. Futurity, m odality, and aspect are closely related,
and future time is rendered by means o f m odal auxiliaries or semi
auxiliaries, or by simple present forms or progressive forms.
3.29
‘WUr and ‘shaU’
will or 7/ + infinitive in all persons
i/wz//-i-infinitive (in 1st person only; chiefly BrE)
I willjshall arrive tom orrow
H e’// be here in half an hour
The future and m odal functions o f these auxiliaries can hardly be
separated; but shall and, particularly, will are the closest approxim ation
to a colourless, neutral future. Will for future can be used in all persons
throughout the English-speaking world, whereas shall (for 1st person) is
largely restricted in this usage to southern BrE.
The auxiliary construction is also used to refer to a statement seen
in the past from a point o f orientation in the future:
They will have finished their book by next year
Note
Other modal auxiliaries can have future reference also : ‘He may leave tomorrow* = ‘He will
possibly leave ...*
3.30
^Be going to’ H- infinitive
This construction denotes ‘future fulfihnent o f the present’. Looked a t
more carefully, be going to has two more specific meanings, o f which one,
‘future o f present intention’, is used chiefly with personal subjects:
W hen are you going to get married?
The other meaning is ‘future o f present cause’, which is found with
both personal and non-personal subjects:
She’^ going to have a baby
IVs going to rain
47
Both of these suggest that the event is already ‘on the way’. Be going
to is not generally used in the main clause of conditional sentences,
will!*11 or shall being preferred instead:
If you leave now, you’// never regret it
3.31
Present progressive
3.32
Simple present
3.33
‘Will/shair + progressive
The auxiliary verb construction can be used together with the progres
sive infinitive to denote a ‘future-as-a-matter-of-course’ : willjshall + be +
+ W-ing. The use of this com bination avoids the interpretation (to
which will, shall, and be going to are liable) o f volition, insistence, etc:
He’// do his best (future or volitional interpretation possible)
He’// be doing his best (future interpretation only)
This complex construction can be used to convey greater tact and
consideration than the simple auxiliary construction does:
3.34
‘Be to’ + inflnitive
This expresses (a) arrangement, (b) command, or (c) contingent future:
(a) We are to be married soon
There’i to be an investigation
(b) You m e to be back by 10 o’clock
(c) if he is to succeed, he must work harder
3.35
‘Be about to’ + inflnitive
This construction expresses near future, ie imminent fulfilment:
The taxi is here and we are about to leave
Be ... to may enclose other items such as shortly or soon to provide
a means o f future expression; with other items again {bound, liable,
certain, {un)likely), future expression is overlaid with m odal meaning:
He is certain to address the meeting {= It is certain that he will address ...)
3.36
Future time in the past
Some o f the future constructions just discussed can be used in the
past tense to express tifne which is in the future when seen from a
viewpoint in the past.
49
(1) A U X ILIA RY VERB CONSiTRUCTION with would (rare; Uterary
narrative style)
The time was not far off when he would regret this decision
(2) he going to + IN FIN IT IV E (often with the sense of ‘unfulfilled
intention’)
Y ou were going to give me your address
(3) PAST PRO G RESSIV E
I was meeting him in Bordeaux the next day
(4) be to + IN FIN IT IV E (formal = ‘was destined’, ‘was arranged’)
He was later to regret his decision
The meeting was to be held the following week
(5) he about to (‘on the point o f)
He was about to hit me
Mood
3.37
M ood is expressed in Enghsh to a very m inor extent by the sub
junctive, as in '
So be it then!
to a much greater extent by past tense forms, as in
If you taught me, I would learn quickly
but above all, by means o f the m odal auxiliaries, as in
It is strange that he should have left so early
3.38
The subjunctive
Three categories o f subjunctive may be distinguished:
. (a) The M A N D A TIV E SUBJUNCTIVE in r/it2/-clauses has only one
form, the base (V); this means there is lack of the regular
indicative concord bet>veeri subject and finite verb in the 3rd
person singular present, and the present and past tenses are
indistinguishable. This subjunctive can be used with any verb in
subordinate that-c\?iuses when the m ain clause contains an expression
o f recommendation, resolution, demand, and so on (W e demand,
require, move, insist, suggest, ask, etc that ...). The use of this
subjunctive occurs chiefly in formal style (and especially in AmE)
where in less formal contexts one would rather make use o f other
devices, such as /o-infinitive or should + infinitive:
50
It is necessary that every member inform himself o f these rules
It is necessary that every member should inform himself o f
these rules
It is necessary for every member to inform himself o f these
rules
(b) The FO R M U L A IC SUBJUNCTIVE also consists o f the base (V)
but is only used in clauses in certain set expressions which have
to be learned as wholes (see 7.63):
Come what may, we will go ahead
G od save the Queen!
Suffice it to say that ...
Be that as it may ...
Heaven forbid that ...
(c) The SUBJUNCTIVE were is hypothetical in meaning and is used
in conditional and concessive clauses and in subordinate clauses
after optative verbs like wish. It occurs as the 1st and 3r<i
person singular past o f the verb be, m atching the indicative was,
which is the m ore common in less formal style:
1 were 1
If she < > to do something like that,...
1was I
He spoke to me as if I ¿eaf
[w a y J
I wish } dead
J
Note
Only were is acceptable in ‘As it were’ ( = so to speak); were is usual in ‘If I were you’.
3.39
Modal past
3.40
CAN/COULD
can
could
Note
[fl] Ability can bring in the implication o f willingness (especially in spoken English):
Can ]
^ , V you do me a favour?
CouldJ ^
52
3.41
MAY/MIGHT
may
might
Note
[a] M ay and might are among the modal auxiliaries which involve differences o f meaning in
passing from declarative to interrogative or negative.
[b] There is a rare use o f may as a ‘quasi-subjunctive* auxiliary, eg to express wish,
normally in positive sentences:
May he never set foot in this house again!
3.42
SHALL/SHOULD
shall
53
O f these three meanings it is only the one o f intention that is widely
used today. Shall is, on the whole and especially outside BrE, an
infrequent auxiliary with restricted use com pared with should, will, and would;
will is generally preferred, except in 1st person questions:
Shall/*Will I come at once?
In the first person plural, eg
W hat shall/will we drink?
shall asks for instructions, and will is non-vohtional future (especially in
AmE). Will Ijwe has become increasingly common not only in con
texts o f non-volitional futurity {Will I see you later?), but also in
sentences expressing helplessness, perplexity, etc:
How will I get there? W hat will I do? Which will I take?
This usage is predominantly AmE (though should is commonly preferred)
but examples may be found in BrE too. A similar meaning is also
conveyed by be going to:
W hat are we going to do?
should
(2) ‘Putative’ use after certain ex It is odd that you should say this
pressions, eg: it is a p ity that, to me
I am surprised that (see 11.51, I am sorry that this should have
12.12, 12.17) happened
(3) Contingent use (1st person only fshouldl love to go abroad (if
and especially BrE) in the main ^ [would J we had the chance)
clause ( = would)
(4) In rather formal real conditions If you should change your mind,
please let us know
3.43
W I L l/ w O U L D
will
(1) Willingness. Used in polite re H e’ll help you if you ask him
quests Will you have another cup of cof
fee?
Will you (please, kindly, etc) open the
window?
(2) Intention. Usually contracted ’//; I ’ll write as soon as I can
m ainly 1st person We won’t stay longer than two
hours
54
(3) Insistence. Stressed, hence no V/ He 'will do it, whatever you say
contraction (‘He insists on doing it ...’)
(C f He 'shall do it, whatever you
say = ‘I insist on his doing it ...’)
He 'will keep interrupting me
would
(1) Willingness W ould you excuse me?
(2) Insistence I t’s your own fault; you 'would take
the baby with you
(3) Characteristic activity in the past Every m orning he would go for a long
(often aspectual in effect) walk (ie ‘it was customary’)
John 'would make a mess of it
(informal = ‘It was typical’)
(4) Contingent use in the main clause He would smoke too much if I
of a conditional sentence didn’t stop him
(5) Probability That would be his mother
Note
Volition with preference is expressed with would rather Isooner:
A: Would you like tea or would you rather have coffee?
B: I think I’d rather have tea.
The expression with sooner is informal. *
3.44
MUST
i
(2) = ‘be obliged not to’ : m ustn’t. needn’t ^
don’t have to be back by
are not obliged to j
10 o ’clock
55
(2) (Logical) necessity There must be a mistake
M ust is not used in sentences but: There cannot be a mistake
with negative or interrogative
meanings, can being used instead.
M ust can occur in superficially M ustn’t there be another reason for
interrogative but answer-assum- his behaviour?
ing sentences.
3.45
OUGHT TO
Note
Ought to and should both denote obligation and logical necessity, but are less categoric
al than must and have to. Ought to is often felt to be awkward in questions involving
inversion, and should is preferred. Still less categorical than ought is hadj’d better ¡best
(plus bare infinitive):
A: Must you go?
B: Well, I don’t have to, but I think I’d better (go).
3.46
The tense of modals
Only some o f the modals have corresponding present and past forms:
PRESENT PAST
can could
may could (might)
shall should
w illr 11 would!
must (had to)
— used to
ought to —
need —
dare dared
♦must
*ought to
Yesterday the children ♦needn’t go out and play
?daren’t
dared not
did not dare
m ust
ought to
needn’t
He said the children daren’t go out and play
dared not
didn’t dare
3.47
The perfective and progressive aspects are normally excluded when the
modal expresses ‘ability’ or ‘permission’, and also when shall or will
express ‘volition’. These aspects are freely used, however, with other modal
meanings; eg
He may have missed the train.
He may have been visiting his mother
‘possibility’
He can’t be swimming all day
. He can’t have been working
{
He m ust have left his imibrella on the bus
‘necessity’ I must be dreaming
57